Is this Constructionism? A case of young
children, mathematics and powerful ideas.
Chrystalla Papademetri-Kachrimani, C.Papademetri@euc.ac.cy
Department of Education Sciences,
European University Cyprus
Abstract
In this paper I argue that
Constructionism need not only be about computers and programming. In continuation
to the themes discussed in Constructionism 2010, this paper argues that if
Constructionism is restricted to the use of computers then constructionism
cannot be applied in most educational settings around the world and builds on
the assumption that schools and teachers should not be ignored but rather
supported as to how to design activities that will formally lead children to
powerful ideas. The paper describes an example of a learning experience in an
early childhood education setting in Cyprus were 25 4-to-5 year olds used
objects provided by their teacher to think with about powerful ideas. The learning
experience did not involve computers but had all the main ideas of
constructionism as expressed by Seymour Papert. It is a learning experience
about young children, mathematics and powerful ideas.
Keywords
Young children, mathematics, powerful
ideas
Introduction
As supported by Noss and Hoyles (1996), the
computer has allowed ‘glimpses to new epistemologies’ and ‘opened new windows
on the construction of meanings’. This acknowledgement has always captured for
me the origin of Constructionism but I had never though that it implied that in
order to create learning experiences in a constructionist way you ought to use
computers. I rather believed that it meant that computer-based research showed
that provided sufficiently sensitive techniques are employed, learners might
gain access to and communicate powerful ideas. Isn’t that what Papert (1993) was
talking about after all in ‘Mindstorms’: Children, Mathematics and Powerful
Ideas’? At least that is what I saw through the window which
constructionism opened for me.
If we manage to move away from ‘teaching
children mathematics’ to ‘teaching children how to think as mathematicians’
(Papert, 1972) isn’t that learning in a constructionism sense?. If we manage to
support children’s learning-by making and thinking-as-constructing (Papert,
1991) and providing children with objects-to-think-with (Papert, 1993) isn’t
that what Constructionism is all about? Almost two years after Constructionsim
2010 I still hear voices in my head concerning what Constructionism is (or
better say, is not). Should Constructionism be restricted to the use of
computers and processes involving programming, or not? In the past, there have
been examples of research within the constructionism paradigm that did not
involve the use of computers (Papademetri, 2007).
If Constructionism is restricted to
computers and programming then it is impossible to apply it to most educational
settings around the world. In this paper I would like to describe a learning
experience from an early childhood education setting in Cyprus. All public kindergarten classrooms in Cyprus have up to 25 3-to-5
year olds under the responsibility of one teacher. There is one computer in
each classroom where the children can play normally during free play. The
teachers are rarely and purely educated as to how to use the computer. Besides the restrictions of the setting in which the learning
experience described in the following sections of this paper occurred, I would
like to argue that this is a learning experience in a constructionist sense.
Methodology
The data presented in this paper originated from the implementation of an activity sequence in a
public kindergarten in Cyprus. The activity sequence was implemented by a
senior student-teacher in a classroom of 25 4-to-5 year olds in a public school
within the children’s everyday program. Children’s time in public schools in
Cyprus is shared between play time and whole classroom activities.
The activity sequence was designed as part,
and in support of a much broader research project involving planning,
implementing, evaluating and scientifically justifying a joint mathematics and
science literacy curriculum for early childhood education, comprising by six
common learning axes (experiences, scientific thinking skills, scientific
thinking processes, attitudes, conceptual understanding, and epistemological
awareness). This three year research project will end in August 2014. The joint
curriculum is developed by a mixed group of researchers, content-knowledge
specialists and educators, based on a review of existing literature and
applications in authentic early childhood settings.
The task sequence was based on the
following problem: ‘How many
different shapes can you make by putting together two congruent scalene,
right-angled triangles so that one pair of congruent sides is always shared?’ The
idea for this problem originated by Claus (1992). In Figure 1 we illustrate all
the solutions to the problem.

(a) (b) (c)
(d) (e) (f)
Figure 1. The solutions to
the problem.
The implementation of the activity sequence
was videotaped and then transcribed. The transcript was then analysed in terms
of the six learning axes. To be more precise, the effort was to identify among
the data the learning outcome of the activity sequence in terms of the
experiences the children gained, the scientific thinking skills, attitudes,
conceptual understanding and epistemological awareness they developed and the
scientific thinking processes involved. For the purposes of this paper we will focus
also to those parts of children’s learning that can be connected to the
constructionism paradigm.
The activity sequence and results from
the implementation
In this section we provide a description of
the task sequence along with data from the implementation.
Activity One: Introduction to the
problem
The student-teacher found an interesting
way to get the children engaged in the problem described in the previous
section. Besides the word triangle, none of the other mathematical terms of the
problem was used to introduce the problem to the children. The children were
asked through an interesting story to find how many different shapes they could
make by using two given triangles and the problem was explained through an example
of an acceptable solution and an example of an unacceptable solution. Through a
discussion she had with the children, the student-teacher made sure that the
children had understood the elements of the problem.
Activity Two: Experimenting and tracing
solutions to the problem
At first the children worked in pairs were
they experimented with a pair of the two congruent scalene, right-angled
triangles while trying to find different solutions to the problem. The children
were asked to find a way to remember their solutions so they decided that they
had to trace each solution on a piece of paper (Figure 2).

Figure 2. The children are
experimenting in order to find the solutions to the problem.
Activity Three: Presenting solutions to
the classroom
Then the children presented their solutions
to the whole of the classroom and through their drawings they concluded that
they had found five different solutions to the problem altogether which they
reproduced with the use of pairs of congruent scalene, right-angled triangles (Figure
3). The children did not identify among their drawings the solution illustrated
in Figure 1(b).
Figure 3. The five solutions
found by the children.
The children along with the student-teacher
wondered whether there were more solutions to the problem. The student-teacher told
the children that they would try and see if there are more solutions to the
problem the next day and took photos of each solution found.
Activity Four: Observing the set of
congruent triangles
The following day the children observed the
two triangles they had used the previous day and made different observations.
They observed that for each side of one triangle they could find the same side
in the other triangle. Thus, as concluded by the children, the two shapes were ‘the
same’. They also observed that the three sides of each triangle were different
between them. When the children were asked to find a way to show which side of
one triangle was the same with which side of the other triangle they decided to
use a different color marker to mark each set of equal sides. After they marked
the two triangles the way they had decided, the teacher showed the children
sets of triangles which she had marked earlier using blue, red and green.
Activity Five: Reproducing and observing
the solutions to the problem
Then the student-teacher showed the
children the photos from the solutions to the problem they had found the
previous day and the children recognized that these were indeed their
solutions. The children were separated into 5 groups. The student-teacher gave
each group a photograph of one of the solutions they had found the previous day
and 2 marked triangles. Each group used the triangles to reproduce the solution
in their photograph. Then the children observed the 5 solutions found, as these
were reproduced using the marked triangles (Figure 4).

Figure 4. The children are
observing their solutions as reproduced with marked sets of triangles.
The student-teacher chose the solution
illustrated in Figure 1c to start the following discussion with the children:
1 |
Teacher: |
What do you observe about the sides you put together in order to
make this shape?(Figure 5a) |
2 |
Child: |
The two sides are open. One is right and one is left. |
3 |
Teacher: |
What do you mean they are open? Explain to me. |
4 |
Child: |
They are like the wings of a bird! |
5 |
Teacher: |
A! The shape you made looks like wings. (Figure 5b)
What else do you observe about the sides you put together? |
6 |
Child: |
They are the small ones. |
7 |
Teacher: |
They are the small ones. What else? |
8 |
Child: |
This shape when you put it the other way round looks like a tear. |
9 |
Teacher: |
But in order to make this shape you put together two sides? The
two sides are different? |
10 |
Child: |
Yes. Because this goes straight down and this goes straight up. (The
child points out one set of parallel sides of the shape, Figure 5c) |
11 |
Teacher: |
Nice? But I am talking about the sides the children put together.
….Observe their color. |
12 |
Child: |
They are both blue. |
13 |
Teacher: |
So they are … |
14 |
Child: |
...the same |
15 |
Teacher: |
Do you see another shape which has the two blue sides joined? |
16 |
Child: |
This one. (Figure 5d) |
17 |
Teacher: |
How come we have two shapes with the blue sides joined? |
18 |
Child: |
They were like this…(The child positions her two hands as shown in
Figure 5e) |
19 |
Teacher: |
And then ….. |
20 |
Child: |
…… the other way round. (The same child as before flips one of her
two hands over) |
21 |
Teacher: |
This looks like wings and this looks like a tear. How can we make
the wings look like a tear? |
22 |
Child: |
(A child after turning the one of the two triangles around for a
while f l i p s it over.) (Figure 5e) |
23 |
Teacher: |
What did you do? |
24 |
Child: |
He turned it upside down. |

(a ) |

(b) |

(c) |

(d) |

(e) |

(f) |
|
|
|
|
|
Figure 5. The children are
discussing one of the solutions of the problem.
The student-teacher proceeded the same way with the two
shape-solutions where the children put together the sides marked red and the
one shape-solution where the children put together the sides marked green. |
25 |
Teacher: |
How many times did we join the red sides? |
26 |
Child: |
Two |
27 |
Teacher: |
How many times did we join the blue sides? |
28 |
Child: |
Two |
29 |
Teacher: |
How many times did we join the green sides? |
30 |
Child: |
One |
31 |
Teacher: |
So what do you observe? …… We have two solutions with blue, two
solutions with red and one solution with green. Now open your ears because I
have a question for you. Do you think we have
found all the solutions? |
32 |
Child: |
No |
33 |
Teacher: |
How many more solutions are there?
……. |
34 |
Child: |
There is one more solution. |
35 |
Teacher: |
Why? |
36 |
Child: |
Because … |
37 |
Teacher: |
Let’s think. Why is there one more? … How many solutions do we
have for each color? |
38 |
Child: |
Two. |
39 |
Teacher: |
Two. Which color doesn’t have two solutions? |
40 |
Child: |
The green one. |
41 |
Teacher: |
So, do you think there is one more solution for… |
42 |
Child: |
...for green. Green is also two. |
43 |
Teacher: |
Which is the solution missing? How can I have a different solution
with the green? |
Two of the children tried to transform the 5th solution into
another shape. Finally they flipped one of the two triangles over thus
discovering the missing solution to the problem] ( Figure 6) |
44 |
Teacher: |
So how many solutions do we have now? |
45 |
Child: |
Six. |
Figure 6: The children are trying to find the missing
solution
Findings
In Table 1 we can see the learning outcomes
which were identified through the data collected from the implementation of the
activity sequence.
Learning Axes |
Learning Outcome of the activity |
Corresponding Activity- Evidence |
Experiences |
You can make different shapes by
combining two other shapes in different ways
Two shapes might have their corresponding
sides equal
There are triangles which have three
unequal sides
Some problems have a specific number of
solutions (there is a reason why the number is specific)
|
Activity Two
Activity Two/Four
Activity Two/Four
Activity Five
|
Scientific Thinking Skills |
Collection of data
Collection of observations
Interpretation of observation
Formulation of a hypothesis |
Activity Two
Activity Four/Five
Activity Five
Activity Five |
Scientific Thinking Processes |
Problem solving
Mechanistic reasoning
|
Activity One-Five
Activity Five |
Attitudes |
Experimentation
Collaboration
Continuation for the completion of a
process
|
Activity One
Activity One
Activity Three-Five |
Conceptual Understanding
|
Congruent triangles |
Activity Four
|
Epistemological Awareness |
Mathematical knowledge is based on
empirical data and observations |
Activity One-Five
|
Table 1. Tracing the learning outcomes of the
activity sequence
In the previous section of this paper, we
can see how young children were involved in a problem-solving activity were
they had to construct shapes by using two congruent triangles, share and
reflect upon their constructions. Reflecting upon constructions is a major
issue within Constructionism (Kafai & Resnick, 1996; Papademetri, 2007;
Resnick, 2007). Through the process and based on the analysis provided in Table
1, the children gained specific mathematical experiences, developed scientific
thinking skills, attitudes, conceptual understanding about congruent shapes and
their epistemological awareness and got involved in scientific thinking
processes.
As far as experiences are concerned, this
is a fundamental learning axis for the education of young children. As pointed
out by Richard Noss during the Constructionism 2010 conference as part of the
‘Constructionism Under Construction’ Panel, ‘engaged in constructionist
activities makes it easier for teachers to teach difficult ideas later on’. In
rephrasing this I would like to support the point of view that experiences are
the basis and a prerequisite for conceptual understanding.
The children were involved in processes of
flipping and rotating shapes (processes which remind us of Dynamic Geometry)
while experimenting in their effort to find different solutions to the problem
and in the process of trying to make and interpret their observations.
Additionally these processes of observing and interpreting their observations (Activity
five, discussion, lines 15-20) which led them to a formulation of a hypothesis
(‘there is one solution missing’ ‘because green also has to be two’) is a
process described by Russ et al (2008) as mechanistic reasoning.
Using a framework derived from the
philosophy of science, Russ et al. (2008) developed a coding scheme of 7 major
components of mechanistic reasoning that can be used to identify and assess
children’s use of mechanistic reasoning. Those components include (i)
descriptions of the target phenomenon (what we see happening), (ii)
identification of the set-up conditions that are necessary for the phenomenon
to happen, (iii) identification of entities (conceptual or real objects) that
play a particular role in the phenomenon, (iv) identification of the entities’
activities that cause changes in the surrounding entities, (v) the entities’
properties, (vi) the entity organization (how entities are located, structured
or oriented within the phenomenon), and (vii) chaining; that is using knowledge
about causal structure to make claims about what has happened prior to a
phenomenon and what will happen.
Based on Russ et all’s (2008) coding scheme
for identifying mechanistic reasoning we can identify the components of
mechanistic reasoning in the learning experience described earlier. The
children made observations (described what they saw happening) – there are two
solutions for the same set of congruent sides (Russ et all’s (2008) component
i). In order to do that, the children identified the data of the problem
(identified the conditions and described the entities which played an important
role in the phenomenon) – this phenomenon arose while trying to construct
shapes by putting together two congruent scalene triangles so that one pair of
congruent sides is completely shared (Russ et all’s (2008) component ii-vi).
After the children described what they saw happening they became involved in a
chaining procedure where they tried to interpret their observation which
allowed them to formulate a hypothesis in relation to the missing solutions to
the original problem – there are two solutions for each set of congruent sides
which result when flipping over one of the two triangles, thus the total number
of solutions to the problem must be 6 (Russ et all’s component vii). This
process of observing something interesting happening and trying to interpret
this observation is so familiar when thinking about children playing with
computers and observing interesting things happening on the screen within the
constructionism research paradigm.
In concluding with the findings, I would
like to pinpoint the ways in which the objects provided to the children
operated as communicative and meaning construction tools. In Papademetri (2007)
through a focused investigation of young children’s understandings of squares
we concluded that ‘in the process of the tasks (designed for the aforementioned
research) the children articulated, through the language provided by the
setting, rich intuitive understandings about the structure of squares and were,
at the same time able to situate their abstractions in the context of
construction’. In the study by Papademetri (2007) the children went through a
three phase task sequence consisting by a Description Task (the children were
involved in classification and shape recognition tasks), a Construction Task
(the children were asked to construct squares with the use of sticks) and a
Reflection Task (the children were asked to reflect on the construction
process). Even though during the Description Task, the children as supported by
existing research exhibited limited understanding about squares, through their
involvement in the Construction Task, they exhibited much richer intuitive
structural understandings. And in the Reflection Task even though the children
in a great extent failed to express about the structure of squares in formal
ways they expressed about the structure of squares in diverse and inventive
ways. As concluded by Papademetri (2007) ‘construction became the language the
children could ‘speak’ and the adult (researcher/teacher) could ‘hear’.
Similarly, in the task sequence described in this paper and through the
children’s involvement we can see how the objects provided gave the opportunity
to the children to think and communicate about powerful ideas without having to
use formal language which is strange to young children. If we go back to
Activity Four (observing the set of congruent triangles) we can see how the
objects provided and the context of the activity allowed the children to think
about and communicate their conceptual understanding of congruent shapes and
scalene triangles. Similarly in Activity Five in line 10 of the discussion
described in this paper we observe how the child refers and expresses his
observations about the two parallel/equal sides of the parallelogram (Figure
1c) they constructed with the two congruent triangles. Thus here we have an
example of the ways in which construction allows young children to think about,
talk about, and reflect on mathematical concepts and phenomena.
Discussion
During the Constructionism 2010 conference
as part of the ‘Constructionism Under Construction’ Panel, Richard Noss
pointed out that ‘powerful ideas mostly can’t be learned by accident’ stressing
out the need for designing activities which will lead learners to powerful
ideas. Furthermore this paper builds on the conviction ‘that
studies in mathematics education should involve some discussion of mathematical
activity, however this is defined’ (Hoyles, 2001).
In this paper we have described one such
activity sequence and have argued that it is characterized by the main aspects
of the constructionist approach. The children were given
an-object-to-think-with, developed their scientific thinking skills and thus
were taught ‘how to think as mathematicians’ learned through construction and
reflection and gained access to powerful ideas. Based on Celia Hoyles comment
in the Constructionism 2010 ‘Taking Stock’ Discussion on assessment I can
retrieve a substantial number of educational activities and pieces of research
using computers (and claiming to be constructionism) that do not involve
learning in a constructionist sense. Thus, the learning experience described in
this paper is one example of how Constructionism can reach teachers and schools
even in those cases were the conditions are not ideal and learning is not
computer-based.
I would like to conclude this discussion
with one final comment. According to Resnick (2007) ‘Kindergarten is undergoing
a dramatic change. For nearly 200 years, since the first kindergarten opened in
1837, kindergarten has been a time for telling stories, building castles,
drawing pictures, and learning to share. But that is starting to change. Today,
more and more kindergarten children are spending time filling out phonics
worksheets and memorizing flashcards. In short, kindergarten is becoming more
and more like the rest of the school.’ We value Renick’s conviction that ‘exactly
the opposite is needed: instead of making kindergarten like the rest of school,
we need to make the rest of school (indeed, the rest of life) more like
kindergarten.’ Resnick (2007) is inspired by Kindergarten’s
traditional-authentic approach to learning in trying to formulate a Lifelong
Kindergarten education. Because it is indeed a fact that Kindergarten is
undergoing a dramatic (and quite sad) change, constructionism can replace
kindergarten’s lost identity and character. Thus it is time for early childhood
education to be inspired (as paradoxical as it sounds) by constructionism.
References
Claus, A. (1992). Exploring Geometry. Arithmetic
Teacher, 40(1), 14-17.
Hoyles, C. (2001). From Describing
to Designing Mathematical Activity: The Next Step in Developing Social Approach
to Research in Mathematics Education? Educational Studies in Mathematics,
46 (1-3), 273-286.
Kafai, Y. B. & Resnick, M. (1996). Introduction. In Y. B. Kafai & M. Resnick (Eds.), Constructionism in Practice: Designing,
Thinking and Learning in a Digital World (1-8). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Noss, R. & Hoyles, C. (1996). Windows
on Mathematical Meanings. Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
Papademetri, C. (2007). Structural
Knowledge about Shapes: A Case Study of Young Children Describing, Constructing
and Reflecting on Squares. PhD Thesis, University of London, Institute of
Education, London, U.K.
Papert, S. (1972). Teaching Children to
be Mathematicians versus Teaching Children about Mathematics. In A. Floyd
(Ed.), Developing Mathematical Thinking (235-245). Wokingham Addison –
Wesley.
Papert, S. (1991). Situating Constructionism. In I. Harel & S. Papert (Eds.), Constructionism (1-12). Norwood: Ablex
Publishing Corporation.
Papert, S. (1993). Mindstorms:
Children, Mathematics and Powerful Ideas (Second Edition). New York: Basic
Books.
Russ, R., Scherr, E., R., Hammer, D.,
& Mikeska, J.(2008) Recognizing mechanistic reasoning in scientific
inquiry. Science Education 92(3), 499-525.