Towards Turing Teaching
Micheál
Ó Dúill, logios.org@googlemail.com
Logios.Org
Abstract
The technicity thesis and its T/V
concept quality division is applied to education and three modes of learning,
based on the medium used, derived. The medium of the mode designated Turing,
has the computer as its medium. Primary school, being both the foundation and cognitively
the most complex phase of education, is considered from traditional and Turing
teaching perspectives. Proto-Turing practice is exemplified. Given that current
method is inefficient and abusive, and transition to Turing teaching is inevitable,
an approach method development is suggested.
Keywords (style: Keywords)
Technicity, teaching, primary school,
concepts, mode, medium, method,, Turing machine.
Introduction
The relationship of computer technology to
teaching in schools is confused. The commonly used term “ICT” (information and
communication(s) technology) has been unhelpful and was recently roundly
criticised as meaningless by the Royal Society (Furber 2012). The Cambridge
Review of Primary Education (Alexander 2010) also found great difficulties with
this curricular area. The author has always viewed such terms as administrative
language, preferring to see the computer and its associated technologies as a
new teaching medium with novel capabilities. Given that this medium can also
emulate extant media, a term was required that clearly specified teaching that
made use of the new capabilities. Given that, conceptually, the learner is
interacting with a Turing machine, the term “Turing teaching” was proposed (Ó Dúill 2011a).
When a new term is introduced it is
essential to try and make its meaning is unambiguous. For Turing teaching which
uses Turing media, disambiguation is achieved by reference to the concept of
technicity and the two qualities of concept that flow from this neurological
adaptation. Technicity as a concept has been developed in the
Eurologo/constructionist community (Ó Dúill 2010), reaching its final
form in a companion paper submitted to Constructionism
2012.
On this foundation a sound, scientific
basis for education may be built. This includes recognition that:
§ the
primary phase is cognitively the most complex and that, at present, deep
understanding resides only in the unarticulated expertise of the primary school
teacher;
§ medium
and method have a specific relationship with mental processes, which determines
learnability and, where concepts collide, may be a cause of cognitive confusion
and conflict;
§ positive affect is crucial to the educational
process: dislike signals cognitive difficulties.
Three modes of education are derived from
medium considerations; two extant, the third Turing. These offer a new
perspective on some well known difficulties that inhibit development of both
literacy and numeracy; and challenge certain strongly held views and
constructivist approaches.
With these considerations in mind, the
character of Turing teaching begins to emerge: working with a medium that has
the capacity to be attuned to the workings of the mind and thereby offers a
more conducive, less oppressive educational experience. This notwithstanding,
Turing teaching has prerequisites: the basics are outlined and exemplary
practice identified. These considerations throw into sharp relief the
difficulties, conceptual and practical, that transition from traditional to
Turing medium in (primary) school entails: a challenge not posed for over six
millennia.
Technicity
The technicity thesis proposes that the
human capacity for technology came about by a very small extension of
connectivity between prefrontal cortex and the rest of neocortex. The
evolutionary history of hominines involved a large increase if brain size,
reaching a maximum of some 1400cc in the Neanderthal. A significant factor in
this increase was relatively greater enlargement of the prefrontal area, which
is the site of working memory and has an executive function. It is the seat of
creativity, gathering information from memory (other neurone circuits) and
recasting it in a process of exploring alternative plans of action. It
modulates and modifies the actions of older parts of the brain. This modulation
is achieved by the massive invasion of most other parts of the brain by
prefrontal neurones. Note that the evolutionary process of prefrontal expansion
and invasion has no teleological aspect. Neuronal expansion, like any other
cellular rebalancing will become genetically fixed if and only if, post facto,
it is adaptive. For neurones this means that for any expansion to become genetically
fixed the information prefrontal neurones source from the rest of the brain
must turn out to be useful to the organism. An extension of this invasive
process to primary sensory cortex is proposed by the technicity thesis.
Information quality
Certain areas
of primary sensory cortex are particularly interesting because of the quality
of the information structurally embedded in their neurone circuits. Information
may be incorporated in neurone circuits for two purposes: instinctive behaviour;
and processing sensory information. The former has a level of complexity
commensurate with the environment. The latter reflects the way the sensory
system processes incoming information. This processing proceeds by
reconstructing the environment from inbuilt elemental information. This is
obvious once it is understood that, for example a photon reacting with a
receptor in the eye results in a nerve impulse. The correct interpretation of
this impulse, formally equivalent to a symbol on a Turing tape, requires the nervous
system already to possess information about colour in order to match the
incoming symbol and reconstitute its physical referent. Because the nervous
system is built on information about properties of matter available to the
genome, these computational units express genomic information; in the example,
on photon frequency in the visible light range. By extending their range of information
sources to primary sensory cortex, prefrontal neurones opened a window onto the
information possessed by the genome, as expressed in the neural processing
system. This information is simple in form and therefore of far lower entropy
than the environmental input to the sense organs so, by definition, more
powerful. Some of the information available at primary sensory cortex, from
which technology and art are constructed, is listed in table 1.
Colour |
Line |
Motion |
Pitch |
Chemical |
Pigment
Art
Spectrum Photons |
Shapes
Architecture Symbols Geometry |
Projectiles Choreography Machines Entropy |
Tone
Music
Time Relativity |
Flavour
Cuisine
Molecules Particles |
Table 1. Some sources of
genomic information expressed in neurone circuits and behavioural correlates.
Note the relationship with the aspects of
child development so clearly seen in kindergarten and absence of a direct link
to language.
Provided that an organism can make use of
this information in a way that proves to be adaptive, the genetic organisation
that underpins it will be retained. It is self evident that this has proved to
be so for the human technicity adaptation.
Two qualities of concept
The information available at primary sensory
cortex is very simple. From the image of a tree at the eye, a complex percept
is created. From simple straight lines at different angles sourced from the
so-called feature detectors, a simple form like a square may be constructed.
This concept may be superimposed on the environment to organise it. For
example, a square may be folded from a roughly torn piece of paper. However,
this form has characteristics that differ from that of a tree. A tree leaning
at a forty five degree angle remains a tree. Rotate a square by the same amount
and it is perceived as a different object: a diamond. This indicates that there
are two routes to concept formation, leading to concepts of differing quality.
The first, characterised by the
square/diamond, is the normal perceptual route. Such concepts are naively
congruent with the environment, and of commensurate entropy. Expressed with
language, with which they co-evolved, they are socio-perceptual. Vigotsky
described this route to concept formation, including its associated
internalised speech. The term V-concept is used to denote it.
The second, characterised by the ideal
square, is the technicity route. In this case the information source is genomic
information expressed in neural structures and directly accessed by prefrontal
cognitive processes. The concepts are prefrontal creations unconnected to
perception or language. The simple elements from which they constructed are of
low entropy, commensurate with that in the genome, and thereby they are
powerful relative to their perceptual counterparts. In describing the eponymous
machine, Turing was employing this mode of conceptualisation. It is the source
of troubling notions such as Platonic ideals and counterintuitive scientific
theories. Derived from the technicity adaptation it is uniquely human and
expressed as inspectable physical constructions. The term T-concept is used to
denote it.
The crucial difference between the
two is that, whilst a V-concept may be accepted because it has internal
linguistic consistency, T-concepts must be shown to be consistent with the
behaviour of the physical world; to which technicity uniquely provides the
human with cognitive access.
Three modes of education
The technicity adaptation, though
genetically inbuilt, differs from genetic predispositions such as language. Speech
begins to come on stream in infancy and is learned through immersion in a
language community and with caregiver tutoring. Technicity, as evidenced by
drawing, begins to emerge somewhat later and is as much a personal exploration
of the attributes of the medium as a means of expression. Unlike language,
which is equally expressive regardless of the culture that owns it, the
technological sophistication of societies has varied markedly. Thus, immersion
in the domestic culture is inadequate for the effective development of
technicity capability and a formal system of education becomes necessary. Based
on the two qualities of concept outlined above, it is possible to derive three
modes of teaching and learning.
The first mode is purely V-conceptual. It
makes use of capabilities that evolved in the primate and hominine lineages and
which reached their apogee in the twin species of Neanderthal and human. These
are: a) spoken language communication; and b) a capacity to learn by
observation from demonstration: learning by rote, repetition, reproduction, and
by inner-speech rehearsal. This mode is sufficient to conserve the knowledge
base and provides a platform for some limited innovation. It was well described
by Vigotsky (1962) and will be denoted by the term Vigotskian.
The second mode is technicity-based and
makes use of graphic forms both to express T-concepts and to provide external
memory storage. The most important innovation is the development of systems of
notation that provide insight into mental processes. Written language and
numerical notation are the foundation of this mode. The former is a technology
that notes the grammatical and lexical aspects of speech, omitting all prosody.
The latter, in its decimal place-value form represents the way humans think
about number. This education system, which Alexander (2010) so fully describes,
has been in use for at least five millennia. The key that unlocks access to the
knowledge base is literacy and numeracy, the apprenticeship in the 3Rs that
dominates current primary education practice. It is an onerous apprenticeship.
Not all children master the grammar of the medium and there are collateral
casualties as a consequence. This mode of schooling has a name with historical
depth. The institution where the grammar is mastered is a Grammar school.
The third mode,
the subject of this paper, barely exists. Its medium is the Turing machine and
it brings into focus those constructional aspects of education that have been
relegated to technology and art in current school curricula. The Turing
machine, by definition, can read, write, and, with a little instruction, do
arithmetic. Represented in classrooms by a stored program digital computer,
this medium has the capacity to assist children in mastering the grammar and
the animation of its content. Text in this medium, which stares silently from
the page of a book, may be animated in a multitude of ways. It relationship with
cognitive processes differs from that of text. The medium conceptually being a
Turing machine, the term Turing teaching will denote it.
Vigotskian |
Grammar |
Turing |
Socio-verbal / observational |
Textual |
Computational |
Shared with Neanderthal |
Uniquely human |
Uniquely human |
No external medium |
Externalised memory |
Externalised processing |
High memory load |
Demanding apprenticeship |
Assistive |
Environmental entropy |
Mixed entropy |
Genomic entropy |
V-conceptual |
V/T-conceptual |
T-conceptual |
Table 3. Some major differences
between the three modes of learning now available to the human.
The main differences between the three
modes of teaching and learning: Vigotskian, Grammar and Turing, are listed in
table 3.
Technicity
thinking
During the primary school years children
learn increasingly to apply the capabilities of technicity. Therefore the
purpose of education, often defined culturally, may now be given a more species
oriented perspective: the development of T-conceptual thinking. The starting
point for this is the V-concepts that children bring to school (Bransford et al
2000). At present this transition begins with the literacy and numeracy.
Children learn to use the power of letters, numbers and shape to bring speech
and perception under T-conceptual control. An important question in terms of
the need for transition to Turing teaching is, “How well does Grammar school
method work?”
Literacy
At first sight it is reasonable to take
spoken language as the starting point for reading and writing. There is a
problem, however. Text does not represent speech. Computer speech engineers
have shown that only the lexical and grammatical aspects of speech are
represented in writing and the sounds and music of speech, the prosody, is not
(Taylor 2008). From a V-conceptual perspective, this is surprising. It seems
obvious that alphabetic writing should represent the sounds of speech.
Reflection shows this cannot be so: There are many dialects of any language and
speech sounds vary enormously. Writing is a technology for noting the unvarying
aspects of a given language and cannot represent such diverse pronunciations as
Cockney and a Scot. This notwithstanding, Grammar schooling does elect to work
from speech to writing: the phonics approach maps letter groups to the sounds
of Received Pronunciation or General American. Mapping between RP or GA and
text is very poor (vide dictionary pronunciation guides). Whilst most children
do learn to read, the misconception that text is speech written down is a major
source of spelling errors and inhibits the writing of many children and adults.
Grammar schooling has no answer to this problem. Administrative and academic
interventions have had minimal effect: teachers have had some six millennia to
hone their technique. Put quite simply: the book is a demanding medium that
offers no help to the child in decrypting and animating its contents or in
creative encryption. The failure rate is high and associated with demotivation
and dislike of school.
Numeracy
The number of children and adults who hate
mathematics is legion. Traditional teaching takes 2 parallel approaches: a) oral,
where children learn the language of number and mentally and orally to compute;
and b) structural, where they count objects and measure. It seems obvious that
these approaches will and do complement each other. Technicity considerations
suggest other wise.
Counting is V-conceptual: a
verbal-perceptual activity. Objects are counted and collected up into base-related
bundles: eggs by the half dozen, Dienes blocks by tens. In the latter case
connection to the fingers of two hands, which are available to mediate
one-to-one correspondence, is clear.
The language of number is T-conceptual. It
represents numbers in a different way and in so doing opens a window on the
working of the mind. Number, though familiar in technological cultures, is not
natural, humans prefer to name people and things, and number words had to be invented.
The language of number appears to mirroring counting; but it does not. Language
works from the absence of anything through enumerated objects up to, but not
including, ten. The brain appears to have a register system, as do real (as
opposed to conceptual) computers. Written language does mirror this way the
mind works but place-value number systems do so with greater clarity.
The cognitive
conflict between the perceptual and linguistic methods is dramatically
illustrated if a chequerboard “hundred-square” is enumerated using decimal place-value
numerals. The first and last lines of such a chequerboard, found in many
primary schoolrooms, are shown in figure 1.
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
91 |
92 |
93 |
94 |
95 |
96 |
97 |
98 |
99 |
100 |
Figure 1. Segment of
chequerboard hundred-square marked with decimal place-value numerals.
The cognitive conflict with language is
clear: humans mentally count to nine and then increment the succeeding register
by one. Place-value numeral systems are a T-conceptual technology that is
representative of mental operations. Perceptual counting is V-conceptual. Here
is a cause for cognitive confusion. By counting and carrying out practical
activities with bundles of ten children do not come to an understanding of
number, already embedded in language, rather the reverse. What they say requires
unambiguous means of external verification. Sawing a unit of abacuses, colouring
hundred squares, and using Dienes blocks for model-making might be a good first
step.
Kitchen maths
The cognitive difficulties that underlie
the failure of Turtle geometry have already been discussed (Ó Dúill 2011b). The same considerations apply to the notion of “kitchen
maths” (Papert 1993,) which, noting the skill with which inumerate individuals handled
quantities and proportions in cooking, sought to find a means of constructing mathematical
ideas from these capabilities. This appears, in principle, to be similar to
working from naïve concepts to scientific ones. However, the danger is of conflating
V-conceptual thinking with the T-conceptual. The key V/T difference is that the
former is derived from perception, from individual experience, and the latter is
constructed from species-level information concerning properties of matter.
T-concepts are superimposed on perceptual experience and not extracted from it.
Whilst direct experience to develop craft skills is possible in the Vigotskian
learning mode, practical experience cannot lead directly to T-conceptualisation.
It is necessary for the teacher to propose such concepts for the learner to project
onto their experience, i.e. the scientific method of checking against reality. Given
that the V/T concept distinction was not available at the time constructionism
was first proposed, some early examples of constructionist practice may require
reconsideration.
Turing teaching
In childhood, Alan Turing played with
Meccano, the mechanical precursor of LEGO Technic. He thereby exercised the
technicity adaptation is ways that Grammar schooling did not encourage. As a
consequence, his PhD on computable numbers could be based on the brilliant
insight that it is not possible to divorce mathematics from the mathematician. The
Turing machine, cf. Carnot’s waterfall image, is based on the image of Turing
himself sitting at his desk with paper, pencil and eraser. The mechanical
Turing can read, write, and erase symbols on an infinite tape; the process of so
doing altering the state of the machine, based on its existing state. At a
stroke of his pen, the second law of thermodynamics was inserted into the field
of mathematics and mathematicians. From an educational viewpoint, the computer
in school is a Turing machine with the capability to read, write, and, with a
little instruction, do arithmetic. This contrasts markedly with text media.
The phrase “with a little instruction” gives
the clue. As Papert so rightly said many years ago and the Royal Society echoed
this year, programmability is an intrinsic property of the medium. This does
not imply that programming is an entrée to mathematics, as the
originators of Logo claimed. It is programmable in the same sense that a
surface is capable of taking a mark. Mark-making on paper is a natural childhood
activity, so is talking. Writing an instruction is a combination of the two;
with the possibility that the medium may respond. Programming is, therefore, no
more an introduction to computer science than is the activity of drawing an
introduction to architecture; and no less so. As a form of writing, there are
necessary precursors to programming. It is the role of the primary phase of
education to establish them in a manner congruent with its curriculum.
Primary precursors
Given that the computer is the medium of
Turing teaching, it follows that mastery of the medium is the first priority. It
is essential that children have one-to-one access to it. The sharing of pencils
is not considered acceptable, no more so is the sharing of keyboards, screens
and graphic input device. New technology is not necessary: any computer less
than twelve years old is suitable.
The start is at the beginning. Primary
school runs from the end of kindergarten to the secondary school. It is the
foundation for all that follows. Like the foundations of a building, the
remainder of education relies on its stability. Similarly, the effort that went
into its construction is invisible in the final product and unconsidered in its
use. A quick resume of its nature is therefore required.
A child entering the primary school years
has a complete language system. Gone are the strange structures of infant
speech, but discoursive speech is still to be formed. Childhood might better be
called chatterhood as children exercise the prime social networking medium of
their species. It is the phase where the technicity adaptation comes strongly
on stream. The beginnings of are seen in the geometrical drawings and primary
colourings of kindergarten. Covering classroom walls, they attest its
importance. Sound making, with voice or instrument; the crude flavours of snack
foods; and choreographed movement in playground games attest its emergence.
Neurologically, this is the period when prefrontal
cortex establishes connections with the rest of the brain. Although the basic
architecture of the nervous system is complete at birth it is pruned and tuned
by experience. A) Orbitomedial fibres find their targets in the older part of
the brain, so moderating and modulating affective factors such as attention,
motivation and emotion. These connections are mature by the onset of puberty at
the end of the primary school years. B) Lateral prefrontal cortex, which
connects to neocortex and is cognitive in function, matures more slowly. The
majority of connective growth takes place during the primary school years
though maturation continues into tertiary education. Primary school experience
is the foundation for all that follows.
Turing basics
Because the Turing medium can carry out
processes, it may be tuned to the mind of the learner. Because the Turing
medium can carry out processes, it may assist the user. Because the Turing
medium can carry out process it can also, and currently mainly does, emulate
non-Turing media. Tuning to the mind and providing assistance have little
explored because both these capabilities conflict with traditional teaching
method. The character of this conflict and the pressing need for its resolution
may be illustrated through literacy and numeracy method. Technicity, recall, is
a constructive cognitive process executed by prefrontal information gathering
and composition.
Writing is a technology that notes only certain aspects of spoken language,
so it is plausible that learning to read and write entails access to
appropriate information in the brain rather than signal processing on perceived
speech. This idea is supported by second language intelligibility studies and
the sign languages of deaf people. Therefore, phonics approaches probably do
not do what is claimed for them. Adults who have spelling difficulties report
using a method that uses a sound model of the text. Though difficult for a
human to articulate,(although children do it in the early stages of reading)
this would be a trivial application within computer speech technology. It would
not be an alternative accent, like RP or GA, more a parallel stream that
reflected the equal weight of letters on the page and the spaces between them;
and thereby more in tune with technicity. The assistive aspect comes from
possibilities that the medium offers for working with text without the need manually
to form letters; keyboarding as a means of playfully to investigating text’s
nature.
Number, as discussed above, is a conceptual mess. Turing media add in the
way the mind does. It follows that playing with the symbols in the medium, not
counting, is a good precursor to mathematical understanding,. There is no
developmental issue in working from the symbols to physical reality. Young
children exercise the ability to create geometric forms from a very early age.
A written number is a shape as much as is a square or a letter; and as learnable.
The problem comes in animating number/operator expressions. The textual method
of Grammar schooling makes this an unnecessarily tedious exercise. As with
reading, the medium is obstructive. Turing media offer a multiplicity of
assistive means of entry into computation. Once learned, the symbol system may
be tested against physical reality: i.e. counting comes after number is
understood. Competence with symbols is the key to higher mathematics.
Medium mastery is a precondition for its effective use. This is as true of Turing
media as pencil and paper. It follows that children need systematically to be
taught to work with the medium from the start of formal schooling. Attainment
expectations need to be determined and set. But there is a very great
difference between mastery of Turing media and mastery of text media. The
medium has the capacity to assist in the process of its mastery. Thus, instead
of tedious practice exercises children may create from the outset. This enables
a graded project-oriented approach to be used. Such and approach was developed
in Bulgaria by a primary school teacher, has been reported to a number of Eurologo
conferences, and is sanctioned for use in primary schools in that country and
Macedonia (Ilieva 2010). It comprises a suite of small programs called
“ToolKID”, written in Comenius Logo, that cover all the possibilities of
working with the computer through a project oriented approach based on the
normal activities for the age of the children (fig.2). In addition to this introductory
software, the curriculum included the option to work with external devices.
This introduced the principles of computer control and Logo programming in a
context that reflected the children’s knowledge of their world using the
language and writing skills they were learning. Additionally, this approach
greatly emphasised the aesthetic aspects of constructional activity.
A five-year-old
who today began to follow this exemplary curriculum would six years later enter
secondary education with new capability. Secondary education would then have
the challenge of new child competences, for example a child whose writing
fluently flows from fingers to screen without a glance at the keyboard; and a
window of six years to adapt. Tertiary education would have a lead-in time of a
dozen years.
Figure 2. The “ToolKID”
suite of programs used to introduce all the possibilities of working with the
computer through a project oriented approach to children in the five to seven
year age range.
The problem is that this curriculum
is not obligatory so only certain children in certain schools develop any real
level of competence. Resources were very much the inhibitor a decade ago but
now computer systems that are perfectly suitable for primary education being thrown
away as users upgrade. Computer supply is not the problem: the problem is perceptual
and conceptual.
Tradition and transition
In the minds of both academics and
educational administrators, including some constructionists, the traditional
medium and methods with which they succeeded are perceived to be superior to
the computer: they clearly exercise the little grey cells. Socrates had similar
objections to writing. A new medium inevitably causes concern, particularly where
it affects high status skills. Hence in literacy and numeracy traditional
method has been prescribed and the computer proscribed. In this climate, the
transition to Turing teaching is inhibited and “ICT” integrated into the
tradition.
A consequence of traditionalism is a
disregard of work children produce using Turing media. An illustrated piece of
writing is devalued as ‘done on the computer,’ a higher quality of thought and
expression notwithstanding. This is illustrative of a factor that might be
termed “academic blight” that currently infects computer application in
education. The fragmented, subject led organisation of academe leads to a partisan
approach to school: their must be in the curriculum. Furber (2012) is
exemplary: A shortage of applicants for computer science motivated an inquiry
into computing in schools by the Royal Society. Primary education merited only
a half page of consideration, and then focussed on the final year; yet Furber,
arrogantly and ignorantly asserted:
“We aspire to an outcome where every primary school pupil
has the opportunity to explore the creative side of Computing through
activities such as writing computer programs (using a pupil-friendly
programming environment such as Scratch).”
The derogatory words
require justification. The problem is the conflation of writing and Scratch.
Scratch uses words on labels that are grammatically colour-coded. The technique
is identical to that used in certain remedial reading approaches; used when
there is a learning difficulty. Scratch serves precisely this role (Wilenski 2010,
Harvey & Mönig 2010). Primary school
children learn to write by constructing words from letters and sentences from
words. Programming is a way of writing a story. The teaching method should not
differ. Scratch (LEGO WeDo software is worse) teaching method introduces
splinter-skill learning, abhorrent to primary education. Here lies the
ignorance; the arrogance is in gratuitously recommending an inappropriate teaching
method to expert professionals. From a Turing teaching perspective, University
is part of the problem.
Risk and technological transitions
are always associated. The transition from atmospheric to high pressure steam
power is an obvious historical example. Development of railways accelerated
only when economic conditions were conducive. More importantly, the engineers
and entrepreneurs who drove the development had little association with
traditional horse powered transport. Rail viability was tested in parallel with
road and canal, not in association. This may offer a model for transition from
text to Turing media in education. It is also clear that change can only begin
in primary school; attempts at secondary level can lead only to the teaching of
splinter skills and to assimilation to traditional conceptual frameworks; or to
disappearance without trace.
Teacher R&D capability, a way forward?
Expertise in the developing child’s mind,
at a level necessary for the effective implementation of innovation, is to be
found only within the primary school teaching profession. To this must be added
mastery of the medium. This suggests an engineering R&D based approach: a
continuous cycle of pilot, assessment and scaling up. The cyclic aspect of the
change process contraindicates the traditional academic project model, which
has not been notably successful. A way forward is an R&D class in school,
publicly or privately funded but independent of both the educational and political
establishment, where a Turing teacher can develop method interactively with
children. A source of technical assistance and software development would be
required. It would help were such classes to operate on a regional basis, cf.
EU Comenius programme, so that teachers and children could interact with
colleagues. This is similar to the R&D model used for ToolKID.
Conclusion
or?
The technicity proposition offers a solid
base for constructionist change in education. Necessarily this starts in the
minds of primary school children. The modes-of-learning analysis shows Turing
teaching to be assistive and tuneable to emerging minds. Traditional method is
obstructive and abusive, leading to an inadequately tamed language instinct. This
reduces capacity for scientific relative to perceptuo-linguistic thought. The
transition to Turing media is inevitable but fractured academe is seen to lack
the necessary catalytic knowledge. Expertise resides, unarticulated, in the
minds of primary school teachers who have mastered the medium. The implication
is that R&D is best carried out in classrooms by practising teachers rather
than through professorial projects. The original idea of Eurologo was to bring
teachers and academics together as equals. It has failed to.
Acknowledgements
Vessela Ilieva of
NSOU “Sofia” in Bulgaria, for insights into working with computers in primary
school, exemplary teaching method, and understanding of language, that
catalysed the notion of Turing teaching.
References
Alexander, R. (ed.): Children, their World, their Education: Final report and recommendations of
the Cambridge Primary Review, Routledge, London (2010).
Bransford, J. D.,
Brown, A. L., Cocking, R. R. (eds.): How People Learn Expanded Edition. National Academy Press, Washington DC (2000).
Furber, S., (ed.): Shut down or Restart? The way forward for computing in schools. The
Royal Society, London (2012).
Harvey, B &
Mönig, J. (2010): Bringing “no ceiling” to Scratch: Can One Language
Serve Kids and Computer Scientists? In: Clayson, E.J., Kalaš, I, Eds.
Proceedings of Constructionism 2010 – 12th European Logo Conference. Faculty of
Mathematics, Physics and Informatics Comenius University, Bratislava.
Ilieva, V.: LEGO and
LOGO in the Primary School – a simple way to learn through creation. In:
Clayson, E.J., Kalaš, I, (eds) Proceedings of Constructionism 2010 – 12th
European Logo Conference. Faculty of Mathematics, Physics and Informatics Comenius University, Bratislava (2010)
Ó
Dúill, M. (2010): Can there be a Science of Construction? In:
Clayson, E.J., Kalaš, I, Eds. Proceedings of Constructionism 2010 – 12th
European Logo Conference. Faculty of Mathematics, Physics and Informatics Comenius University, Bratislava.
Ó
Dúill, M. (2011a): The Technicity Thesis: De-re-constructing “Integrating
ICT into education.” In: Proceedings of ELDE Conference, Sofia, Bulgaria, 6 – 8 April 2011, pp. 416-423, Demetra, Sofia.
Ó
Dúill, M. (2011b): Logo: A Turtle Tragedy in
Three Acts. In: Kőrösné M. M. & Farkas, K.
(eds) Proceedings of Hungarologo 2011, Budapest, Hungary, June 2011.
Papert, S. (1993). The Children’s
Machine. New York: Basic
Books.
Vigotsky, L.
(1962): Thought and Language. The MIT Press, Cambridge MA.
Wilensky, U.
(2010): Restructurations: Reformulating Knowledge Disciplines through New
Representational Forms. In: Clayson, E.J., Kalaš, I, Eds. Proceedings
of Constructionism 2010 – 12th European Logo Conference. Faculty of
Mathematics, Physics and Informatics Comenius University, Bratislava.